James-logoJames-logoJames-logoJames-logo
  • Journalism
  • Corporate writing
  • Psychology
  • Blog
  • Get in touch
Lockdown easing will kickstart a hat-trick of M&A strategies
27 May 2020
Human perception has a bandwidth problem
1 June 2020

Operations of cognition: understanding the Stroop effect

Published by Cognitive Psychology on 1 June 2020
James Wallace

Almost a century ago, John Ridley Stroop, a pioneering American psychologist, identified an innate feature of human visual processing which provided breakthrough insight into how brains functions.

Namely, our brains make sense of the world around us – in all its simplicity and complexity – in respect of appearance and ascribed meaning. For example, we understand wooden table is made by trees and that honey comes from bees. This cognitive processing is automatic and effortless – in fact, we usually remain blissfully unaware of such background processing. What Stroop managed to demonstrate was to shine a light on how that automatic processing works; to shine a light on something rarely noticeable. And he did so in an experiment celebrated for its simplicity and elegance.

In the classic experiment, the Stroop effect is measured across three conditions: congruent, incongruent and neutral.

In the first condition, the participant is timed reading a series of coloured words which are printed in a different colour to the word itself, such as the word “blue” is printed in red, and “yellow” in printed in green, and so on. In the congruent condition, the word and colour match (i.e. “blue” is printed in blue ink). In the incongruent condition, word and colour are mismatched (i.e. the word “green” is printed in blue ink). In the neutral condition, a distractor word, unrelated to colour, is employed. Stroop interference occurs in the processing of two conflicting attributes of the same stimuli delays reaction time (RT).

In the congruent and incongruent conditions, the words are published in the wrong colour. However, this incongruence is processed differently by the brain. Stroop’s experiment showed that it takes more time to correctly read the colours of the words, than to read the text in which the words are printed (Stroop, 1935).

The Stroop Effect is caused by inference between task-relevant and task-irrelevant dimensions of the same stimulus which requires higher cognitive attention (Stroop, 1935, Scarpina & Tagini, 2017, Banich, 2019). Naming the colours in which words are printed is slowed by incongruent colour words. By contrast, Stroop facilitation can yield faster RTs. This occurs when two sets of conflicting visual information based on the same stimuli creates mutual interference which causes a slower RT in response selection (Scarpina & Tagini, 2017, Dell’Acqua et al., 2007). Incongruent Stroop tasks are less automated which inhibits the suppression of irrelevant information possible during congruent and neutral tasks.

What causes the Stroop effect?

The Stroop effect is the result of the brain encoding and evaluating environmental sensory information brain to determine the most appropriate action or response (Sahinoglu and Dogan, 2016). Since the Stropp effect was first discovered, by John Stroop in 1935, many hypotheses have been offered to explain this stimulus-response relationship. The three most credible explanations are as follows.

First, Fraisse (1969) suggested that the faster pace of word processing interferes with slower pace of colour processing in the brain (Forrin and MacLeod, 2017). Also, incongruent colours are too slow to interfere with word processing in a word reading Stroop task (Forrin and MacLeod, 2017). The hypothesis is well supported. Recent studies have shown that relative speed of processing is mediated by the contingency-learning effect where irrelevant features are processed before relevant features and the properties assigned to the tasks themselves (Forrin and MacLeod, 2017).

Second, Stroop effects result from cascaded information processing (McClelland, 1979). This hypothesis secured modern support by Riès et al. (2012) which supported the notion that information is continuously accumulated through multiple processing stages.  Stroop effects emerge when the final stage of information accumulation has reached a predefined level. However, where a processing stage is manipulated through experimentation, this can influence response RT. The cascade hypothesis implicates concurrent engagement of specific brain regions in cognitive processes as well as in the dynamic interaction between regions (Banich, 2019, Riès et al., 2012).

Third, task conflict triggers the Stroop effect. Executive control, which enables irrelevant information suppression, is unequally allocated in different Stroop tasks. Control is relaxed following congruent stimuli, and engaged after incongruent stimuli (Kalanthroff et al., 2013). Trials which include increased frequency of congruent stimuli can render executive control inactive, enhancing Stroop interference (Kalanthroff et al., 2013). This hypothesis is supported by neuroimaging studies which show the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a brain region associated with conflict management, is more active in incongruent, as opposed to congruent, Stroop tasks (Bench et al., 1993; Carter, Mintun, & Cohen, 1995). Thus, multiple levels of interference and control generates and influences the Stroop effect, rather than the existence of a single locus within the brain (Banich, 2019). Neuroimaging data shows that lateral prefrontal regions are more associated with task-relevant versus task-irrelevant information processing, while cingulate regions are implicated in late-stage response-related aspects of control (Banich, 2019).

What influences the Stroop effect?

Stroop performance is mediated by task type, presence of task conflict, compatibility effects, practice, as well as demographics and individual differences. Two types of conflict mediate the Stroop effect: task and information. Task conflict is between relevant colour naming and irrelevant word reading which can arise when the proactive task-demand control mechanism is reduced (Kalanthroff et al., 2013). Information conflict relates to a conflict between two attributes of the same stimuli (i.e. whether the colour of the ink of the word is congruent or incongruent). Van der Elst et al. (2006) found that speed-dependent, as opposed to accuracy-dependent, Stroop tasks were significantly affected by age, sex and education. Elsewhere, Bugg et al. (2007) identified age differences in Stroop performance, in part, associated with deteriorated inhibitory control. The role of gender is more equivocal; some studies have reported differences (Hameleers et al., 2000; Van Boxtel et al., 2001), while others showed no significance (Klein et al., 1997).

According to the dual mechanisms of control theory (DMC; Braver, 2012), task conflict scenarios produced an increased Stroop effect and a reversed Stroop facilitation (RF) occurs (i.e. faster RT for neutral nonword stimuli than for congruent stimuli). In the Kornblum (1965) experiments, the Stroop effect was mediated by greater competition, or inhibition, between fingers of the same hand than between fingers of different hands. This is due to the mechanically linkage of index and middle finger response selection on the same hand, and mechanical independence when finger response is selected from different hands (Rosenbaum, 2010). Within demographics influences, age was a significant influence on accuracy Stroop tasks among older people with lower education, implying executive functioning deteriorates greater among older people from lower income backgrounds Van der Elst et al. (2006).

Stroop effect and individual differences

Individual difference studies have identified a correlation between the magnitude of the Stroop effect and working memory (WM) capacity (Kalanthroff et al., 2013). Patient populations with executive and frontal deficits demonstrated higher error rates in Stroop tasks than healthy controls, which revealed by the role of of the frontal executive system.

Stroop effect variants

The response-response (R-R) compatibility effect, revealed by Kornblum (1965), showed the Stroop effect is pronounced (i.e. the RT is slower) when participants’ respond by specific fingers on the same hand, rather than different hands (Rosenbaum, 2010). Shiffrin & Schneider (1977) demonstrated that practice increases automatic processing, which can subsequently influence Stroop performance. Negative priming is another variant. This is where a previously ignored stimuli subsequently becomes harder to pay attention to.

References

Banich, M. T. (2019). The Stroop Effect Occurs at Multiple Points Along a Cascade of Control: Evidence From Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches.  Frontiers in Psychology,10, 2,164.   Doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02164

Bench, C. J., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M., Friston, K. J., Paulesu, E., Frackowiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1993). Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the Stroop test. Neuropsychologia, 31(9), 907–922. Doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90147-r

Braver T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(2), 106–113. Doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010

Bugg, J. M., DeLosh, E. L., Davalos, D. B., & Davis, H. P. (2007). Age Differences in Stroop Interference: Contributions of General Slowing and Task-Specific Deficits. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(2), 155-167. Doi:10.1080/138255891007065

Carter, C. S., Mintun, M., & Cohen, J. D. (1995). Interference and facilitation effects during selective attention: an H215O PET study of Stroop task performance. NeuroImage, 2(4), 264–272. Doi:10.1006/nimg.1995.1034

Dell’Acqua, R., Job, R., Peressotti, F. et al. (2007). The picture-word interference effect is not a Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 717–722. Doi:10.3758/BF03196827

Fraisse, P. (1969). Why is naming longer than reading? Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam, 30, 96–103. Doi:10.1016/0001-6918(69)90043-2

Forrin, F. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2018) This time it’s personal: the memory benefit of hearing oneself. Memory, 26(4), 574-579, DOI:10.1080/09658211.2017.1383434

Kalanthroff, E., Avnit, A., Henik, A., Davelaar, E., & Usher, M. (2014). Stroop proactive control and task conflict are modulated by concurrent working memory load. Psychonomic bulletin & review. Doi:0.3758/s13423-014-0735-x.

Kalanthroff, E., Goldfarb, L., Usher, M., & Henik, A. (2013). Stop interfering: Stroop task conflict independence from informational conflict and interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(7), 1356–1367. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.741606

Kornblum, S. (1965). Response competition and/or inhibition in two-choice reaction time. Psychonomic Science, 2(2), 55–56. Doi:10.3758/BF03343326

McClelland, J. L. (1979). On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86(4), 287–330. Doi:10.1037/0033-295X.86.4.287

Riès, S., Legou, T., Burle, B. et al. (2012). Why does picture naming take longer than word reading? The contribution of articulatory processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 955–961 Doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0287-x

Rosenbaum, D., A. (2010). Human Motor Control. (2nd Ed). Academic Press. Doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-374226-1.00009-7.

Sahinoglu, B., & Dogan, G. (2016). Event-Related Potentials and the Stroop Effect. The Eurasian journal of medicine, 48(1), 53–57. Doi:10.5152/eurasianjmed.2016.16012

Scarpina, F., & Tagini, S. (2017). The Stroop Color and Word Test. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 557. Doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557

Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1–66. Doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. Doi:10.1037/h0054651

Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., & Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop Color-Word Test: Influence of Age, Sex, and Education; and Normative Data for a Large Sample Across the Adult Age Range. Assessment, 13(1), 62–79. Doi:10.1177/1073191105283427

Share this post

0 shares
  • Share
  • LinkedIn
  • Tweet
  • Print
Share
Cognitive Psychology
Cognitive Psychology

Related posts

25 November 2020

AC 2020: Europe will not return to pre-pandemic life for two years


Read more
9 November 2020

The return of commercial property distress


Read more
4 November 2020

Tale of two needles: Trump and Biden locked in tight White House race


Read more

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This is an evolving space. Do get in touch if you think I can help.

© 2020 James Wallace - All Rights Reserved